研究者業績

松下 戦具

マツシタ ソヨグ  (Soyogu Matsushita)

基本情報

所属
大阪樟蔭女子大学 学芸学部・化粧ファッション学科 教授
学位
大阪大学博士(人間科学)(2008年3月 大阪大学)

J-GLOBAL ID
201501050322275091
researchmap会員ID
B000244286

外部リンク

 

 

シリコンで作った自分の指です。最近はこういったものを自作しつつ,人工皮膚の見え方の研究もしています。

 

化粧心理学研究室のページ

https://sites.google.com/osaka-shoin.ac.jp/cosmeticpsychology

 


委員歴

 3

論文

 33
  • 松下戦具
    大阪樟蔭女子大学研究紀要 14 224-231 2024年1月  筆頭著者責任著者
  • 松下 戦具, 菊永 佐紀子, 青山 純也, 野村 毅
    日本顔学会誌 23(2) 41-50 2023年2月  査読有り筆頭著者責任著者
  • 松下 戦具, 三島 爽香
    容装心理学研究 1(1) 29-38 2022年5月  査読有り筆頭著者責任著者
  • 松下戦具
    大阪樟蔭女子大学研究紀要 12 215-222 2022年1月  筆頭著者責任著者
  • 真殿 由加里, 松下 戦具
    大阪樟蔭女子大学研究紀要 12 155-160 2022年1月  
  • 松下戦具
    大阪樟蔭女子大学研究紀要 11 67-73 2021年1月  筆頭著者
  • Hiroyuki Muto, Soyogu Matsushita, Kazunori Morikawa
    Imagination, Cognition and Personality 40(4) 351-371 2020年8月20日  査読有り
    Mental rotation is known to be mediated by sensorimotor processes. To deepen our understanding of the role of somatosensory inputs in mental rotation, we investigated the effects of holding weight by the hands on mental rotation performance. In an experiment, 22 male and 22 female students performed a chronometric mental rotation task while holding either light or heavy bags in both hands. Results showed that females holding heavy bags were quicker and more accurate at mental rotation than females holding light bags, as evidenced by shallower slopes for response times (RTs) and error rates. In contrast, males showed no such heavy-bag-induced improvement. Unlike slopes, intercepts for RTs and error rates were equivalent regardless of sex and bag weight. Consistent with previous research on embodied cognition, the present findings demonstrated the facilitatory role of somatosensory cues by weight in mental rotation and suggested sex differences in embodied processes in mental rotation.
  • 松下戦具
    大阪樟蔭女子大学研究紀要 (10) 141-151 2020年1月  筆頭著者責任著者
  • Hiroyuki Muto, Soyogu Matsushita, Kazunori Morikawa
    Cognition 191 Article 103987 2019年10月  査読有り
  • 松下戦具
    大阪樟蔭女子大学研究紀要 9 247-242 2019年1月  筆頭著者責任著者
  • Soyogu Matsushita, Hiroshi Ono
    Perception 48(4) 338-345 2019年  査読有り筆頭著者責任著者
  • 青池広樹, 渕上幾太郎, 上條洋士, 細川博史, 松下戦具, 森川和則
    日本化粧品技術者会誌 52(2) 105-110 2018年6月  査読有り
  • Kobayashi, Y, Matsushita, S, Morikawa, K
    Perception 24(7) 772-779 2018年5月  査読有り
  • Soyogu Matsushita, Kazuki Sato, Kosuke Murakami, Shuma Tsurumi, So Kanazawa, Masami K Yamaguchi
    Scientific Reports 8(1) :3775 2018年2月  査読有り筆頭著者責任著者
  • Muto, H, Matsushita, S, Morikawa, K
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 44(3) 337-355 2018年2月  査読有り
  • Yuki Kobayashi, Soyogu Matsushita, Kazunori Morikawa
    i-Perception 8(4) 1-10 2017年7月  査読有り
    Whereas geometric illusions in human faces have been reported by several studies, illusions of color or lightness in faces have seldom been explored. Here, we psychophysically investigated whether lip color influences facial skin's perceived lightness. Results of Experiment 1 demonstrated that redder lips lightened and darker lips darkened the perceived complexion. These lightness or darkness inducing effects differ from the classical illusion of lightness contrast in nonface objects for two reasons. First, illusory effects are more assimilative than contrastive. Second, the inducing area (i.e., lips) is much smaller than the influenced area (facial skin). Experiment 2 showed that the assimilative lightness induction was caused by holistic processing of faces. This is the first study to scientifically substantiate the claim of cosmetics manufacturers and makeup artists that lip colors can alter perceived facial skin color. Implications for face perception, lightness illusion, and perceptual effects of cosmetics are discussed.
  • Soyogu Matsushita, Shingo Nomura
    Music perception 34(2) 123-131 2016年12月  査読有り筆頭著者責任著者
    GROOVE IS A PLEASANT FEELING THAT COMPELS people to move their bodies along with music. In the past, there was some consensus among both musicians and researchers that the main factor in inducing this feeling is onset asynchrony of sounds. However, recent studies have asserted that no-asynchrony is the condition that will obtain the highest groove. The current study examined whether no-asynchrony exclusively elicits the highest groove. In Experiment 1, we measured the groove increment of a backbeat drum pattern as a function of the asynchronies between bass guitar and hi-hat cymbal sounds. Upon evaluation, the scores of no conditions exceeded those of the synchronous condition. However, the condition with slight bass guitar precedence over the hi-hat achieved an approximately equal score to the condition with complete synchrony, and that score was higher than the ones achieved with bass delay. In Experiment 2, we measured the participants' sensitivities to timing discrimination. The results confirmed that the amount of bass precedence in Experiment 1 was perceptible to the listeners. These findings suggest that complete synchronization is not always the best condition to achieve groove and that listeners prefer perceivable asynchronies in some cases.
  • Soyogu Matsushita, Kazunori Morikawa, Haruna Yamanami
    Journal of Cosmetic Science 66(3) 161-174 2015年7月  査読有り筆頭著者責任著者
  • Kazunori Morikawa, Soyogu Matsushita, Akitoshi Tomita, Haruna Yamanami
    Frontiers in human neuroscience 9 9:139 2015年3月  査読有り
    Does an assimilative illusion like the Delboeuf illusion occur in the human face? We investigated factors that might influence the perceived size of the eyes in a realistic face. Experiment 1 manipulated the position of the eyebrows (high or low), the presence/absence of eye shadow, and the viewing distance (0.6 m or 5 m), then measured the perceived eye size using a psychophysical method. The results showed that low eyebrows (i.e., closer to the eyes) make the eyes appear larger, suggesting that the assimilation of eyes into the eyebrows is stronger when the eye-eyebrow distance is shorter. The results also demonstrated that the application of eye shadow also makes the eyes look larger. Moreover, the effect of eye shadow is more pronounced when viewed from a distance. In order to investigate the mechanism of the eye size illusion demonstrated in Experiment 1, Experiment 2 measured the magnitude of the Delboeuf illusion at a viewing distance of 0.6 m or 5 m, with or without gray gradation simulating the eye shadow that was used in Experiment 1. The experiment demonstrated that the Delboeuf illusion is modulated by viewing distance and gradation in the same way as the eye size illusion. These results suggest that the eye size illusion induced by the eyebrows and the Delboeuf illusion involve the same mechanism, and that eye shadow causes the assimilation of the eyes into itself and enhances assimilation between the eyes and the eyebrows.
  • 松下戦具
    大阪大学大学院人間科学研究科紀要 41 213-228 2015年2月  筆頭著者責任著者
  • Soyogu Matsushita, Kazunori Morikawa, Saya Mitsuzane, Haruna Yamanami
    Perception 44(5) 529-540 2015年  査読有り筆頭著者責任著者
    We investigated whether the position of the eyebrows influences the perceived shape of the eyes by employing psychophysical measurements. Experiment 1 used arched and straight eyebrows at five different inclinations as stimuli and measured the perceived inclination of the eyes. The results demonstrated that the eyes are perceived to be somewhat inclined in the same direction as the eyebrows. Experiment 2 measured the perceived eye size by manipulating the distance between the eyes and the eyebrows and the curvature of the eyebrows across three levels. The results showed that the lower eyebrows (ie closer to eyes) made the eyes appear larger and the higher eyebrows made the eyes appear smaller, while eyebrow curvature had no effect on perceived eye size. Experiment 3 examined the role of the eye eyebrow distance in the eye inclination illusion shown in experiment 1. The eye inclination illusion was unaffected by the eye eyebrow distance, suggesting that the eye inclination illusion and the eye size illusion may involve different kinds of assimilation. These illusions are discussed in terms of face perception and possible practical applications.
  • 山南春奈, 松下戦具, 森川和則
    人間生活工学 15(2) 23-26 2014年9月  査読有り
  • Akitoshi Tomita, Sayumi Yamamoto, Soyogu Matsushita, Kazunori Morikawa
    Japanese psychological research 56(1) 24-32 2014年1月  査読有り
    Recognition memory performance for familiar faces is better than that for unfamiliar faces. However, it is unclear whether this familiarity effect also occurs for unfamiliar faces which resemble familiar faces. In Experiment1, we tested recognition memory for unfamiliar faces, and showed that unfamiliar faces that resemble familiar faces are recognized more accurately than are unfamiliar faces that do not. In Experiment2, we attempted to measure how accurately the degree of resemblance is remembered. We had participants remember unfamiliar faces that resembled either familiar faces or unfamiliar faces. Then the participants were asked to choose the face that they thought they had seen from an array of faces whose degree of resemblance was systematically varied. The results demonstrated that resemblance to familiar faces enhances encoding of facial information, and that the degree of resemblance to familiar faces is exaggerated in memory. Experiment3 indicated that people perceive mild anti-caricatures to be most like the real faces that are familiar to them. This finding suggests that exaggerated resemblance in memory is not due to exaggerated representations of familiar faces.
  • 富田瑛智, 松下戦具, 森川和則
    認知心理学研究 10(2) 151-163 2013年2月  査読有り
  • 松下戦具, 櫻井研三, 矢野澄男, 須佐見憲史, Hiroshi Ono
    基礎心理学研究 31(1) 1-11 2012年9月  査読有り筆頭著者責任著者
    We examined the perception of depth and stability when viewing a video movie made with a camera that moved laterally back and forth 65mm. The two viewing conditions were: (a) head movement synchronized to the stimulus movement and (b) head stationary. In Experiment 1 (N=16), both conditions showed approximately the same magnitude of apparent depth. We argue that (a) in the head movement condition, observers used the cue produced by observer-produced motion parallactic depth, whereas (b) in the no head movement condition, they used the kinetic depth cue or cue(s) from "structure from motion". In Experiment 2 (N=15), the head movement condition showed a greater stability (less perceived motion) compared to the head stationary condition. The results indicate that simulating observer-produced motion parallax (head movement condition) on a video movie of an actual object has no advantages in terms of depth perception. The advantage lies in producing greater stability.
  • 松下戦具, 赤井誠生
    大阪大学大学教育実践センター紀要 7 69-73 2011年3月  
  • 松下戦具, 赤井誠生
    大阪大学大学教育実践センター紀要 6 1-7 2010年3月  筆頭著者責任著者
  • 赤井誠生, 日野林俊彦, 新居佳子, 松下戦具, 野村弘平
    大阪大学大学教育実践センター紀要 5 33-35 2009年3月  
  • 松下 戦具, 赤井 誠生
    基礎心理学研究 27(1) 13-20 2008年  査読有り筆頭著者責任著者
    In this article, we provided a mathematical model for analyzing the distribution of reaction time and estimated which types of visual search processes would be observed, such as memory-driven or memory-free types mentioned in Horowitz and Wolfe (2003). Our model was a convolution of the Weibull and Gaussian distributions (Weibull-Gaussian model). We conducted visual search experiments and model fittings for the obtained data. The model comparison revealed that the Weibull-Gaussian model was more appropriate than the ex-Gaussian model. Applying the model to the data revealed that the parameter indicating the type of process changed within a session. These results indicated that the visual search processes were not fixed in one type.
  • 新居佳子, 赤井誠生, 和田一成, 堀下智子, 松下戦具
    大阪大学大学教育実践センター紀要 3 1-4 2007年3月  
  • 松下戦具, 赤井誠生
    大阪大学人間科学研究科紀要 33 129-141 2007年3月  筆頭著者責任著者
  • 赤井誠生, 和田一成, 堀下智子, 新居佳子, 松下戦具
    大阪大学大学教育実践センター紀要 2 31-34 2006年3月  
  • 和田一成, 赤井誠生, 堀下智子, 新居佳子, 松下戦具
    大阪大学大学教育実践センター紀要 1 1-8 2006年3月  

MISC

 98

書籍等出版物

 3

講演・口頭発表等

 4

共同研究・競争的資金等の研究課題

 8

作成した教科書

 2
  • 件名
    「高等教育論入門 -- 大学教育のこれから」
    年月日(From)
    2010/11/30
    概要
    「高等教育論入門 -- 大学教育のこれから」ミネルヴァ書房 早田幸政・青野透・諸星裕(編著)
  • 件名
    心理学概論:ヒューマン・サイエンスへの招待
    概要
    松田幸弘(編著) ナカニシヤ出版 第2章 感覚と知覚の心理学

その他(教育上の能力)

 1
  • 件名
    高校生に向けた講義など
    概要
    大学生向けだけでなく高校生に向けても,例えば出張講義や,オープンキャンパスでの模擬授業などを行っている。これらの講義は,その後入学した学生から「あの授業が興味深くて志望を固めるきっかけになった」と評されることもあり,評判は良い。

その他(職務上の実績)

 1
  • 件名
    【獲得外部資金】(受託研究費等)PIAS株式会社