研究者業績

唐澤 真弓

カラサワ マユミ  (Mayumi Karasawa)

基本情報

所属
東京女子大学 現代教養学部 心理コミュニケーション学科 コミュニケーション専攻 教授
学位
博士(文学)(白百合女子大学)

J-GLOBAL ID
200901020282835974
researchmap会員ID
1000183091

経歴

 2

論文

 28
  • Ka I Ip, Barbara Felt, Li Wang, Mayumi Karasawa, Hidemi Hirabayashi, Midori Kazama, Sheryl Olson, Alison Miller, Twila Tardif
    Psychological Science 32(7) 998-1010 2021年7月  
    Adults are biologically responsive to context, and their responses to particular situations may differ across cultures. However, are preschoolers’ biological systems also responsive to situational contexts and cultures? Here, we show that children’s neurobiological stress responses, as indexed by salivary cortisol, are activated and responsive to psychosocial stressors relevant to their sociocultural emphases. By examining cortisol changes across different contexts among 138 preschoolers living in the United States, China, and Japan, we found that an achievement-related stressor elicited an increased cortisol response among Chinese preschoolers, whereas interpersonal-related stressors elicited an increased cortisol response among Japanese preschoolers. By contrast, U.S. preschoolers showed decreased cortisol responses after these stressors but consistently higher levels of anticipatory responses to separation at the beginning of each session. Our findings suggest that children’s neurobiological stress systems may be a critical biological mechanism allowing societal-level cultural phenomena to be embodied in individual-level responses, even among preschoolers.
  • Christopher L. Coe, Yuri Miyamoto, Gayle D. Love, Mayumi Karasawa, Norito Kawakami, Shinobu Kitayama, Carol D. Ryff
    Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 2020年8月  
  • Natsu Sasaki, Kazuhiro Watanabe, Kotaro Imamura, Daisuke Nishi, Mayumi Karasawa, Chiemi Kan, Carol Diane Ryff, Norito Kawakami
    BMC psychology 8(1) 75-75 2020年7月20日  査読有り
    BACKGROUND: The aim of this study was to examine the internal consistency, structural validity, and convergent/known-group validity of the Japanese version of the 42-item Psychological Well-Being Scale (PWBS-42). METHODS: The PWBS-42 includes six 7-item subscales designed to measure the following dimensions of eudaimonic psychological well-being: 1) autonomy, 2) environmental mastery, 3) personal growth, 4) positive relations with others, 5) purpose in life, and 6) self-acceptance. A questionnaire was administered to 2102 community residents in Tokyo aged 30 or over as a part of the Midlife in Japan (MIDJA) survey, in 2008. The internal consistency reliability was tested using Cronbach's α. Structural validity was examined using exploratory factor analysis (EFA). Convergent validity was evaluated by calculating correlations of the Japanese PWBS-42 subscales with life satisfaction, negative affect, negative adjectives, positive affect, positive adjectives, self-esteem, and perceived stress scales. RESULTS: Data from 1027 respondents (505 males and 522 females) were analyzed (valid response rate = 56.2%). Cronbach's α values ranged from 0.70 to 0.78 for five of the subscales, while that for purpose in life was lower (0.57). EFA yielded a five-factor structure: The first two factors consisted of negative and positive items mostly from the environmental mastery, purpose in life, and self-acceptance subscales. The third, fourth, and fifth factors consisted mostly of items from the positive relations with others, autonomy, and personal growth subscales, respectively. As hypothesized, the scores for life satisfaction, negative and positive affect/adjectives, self-esteem and perceived stress were significantly correlated with all subscales of the Japanese PWBS-42. CONCLUSION: The subscales of the Japanese version of the PWBS-42 showed accep. levels of reliability and support for convergent validity in the Japanese population. The factor structure was slightly different from the theoretical 6-factor model: items of three subscales (environmental mastery, purpose in life, and self-acceptance) loaded together on two factors. This finding may be interpreted in light of the interdependent self construal found in Japan in which these three components could be closely linked.
  • Magali Clobert, Tamara L. Sims, Jiah Yoo, Yuri Miyamoto, Hazel R. Markus, Mayumi Karasawa, Cynthia S. Levine
    Emotion 20(2) 164-178 2020年3月  
  • Shinobu Kitayama, Jiyoung Park, Yuri Miyamoto, Heiwa Date, Jennifer Morozink Boylan, Hazel R. Markus, Mayumi Karasawa, Norito Kawakami, Christopher L. Coe, Gayle D. Love, Carol D. Ryff
    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 2018年1月30日  査読有り
  • Levine, C. S, Miyamoto, Y, Markus, H. R, Park, J, Kitayama, S, Kawakami, N, Karasawa, M, Boylan, J. M, Coe, C. L, Love, G. D, Ryff, C. D
    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 42 1335-1348 2016年  査読有り
  • Shinobu Kitayama, Jiyoung Park, Jennifer Morozink Boylan, Yuri Miyamoto, Cynthia S. Levine, Hazel Rose Markus, Mayumi Karasawa, Christopher L. Coe, Norito Kawakami, Gayle D. Love, Carol D. Ryff
    PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 26(2) 211-220 2015年2月  査読有り
    Expression of anger is associated with biological health risk (BHR) in Western cultures. However, recent evidence documenting culturally divergent functions of the expression of anger suggests that its link with BHR may be moderated by culture. To test this prediction, we examined large probability samples of both Japanese and Americans using multiple measures of BHR, including pro-inflammatory markers (interleukin-6 and C-reactive protein) and indices of cardiovascular malfunction (systolic blood pressure and ratio of total to HDL cholesterol). We found that the link between greater expression of anger and increased BHR was robust for Americans. As predicted, however, this association was diametrically reversed for Japanese, among whom greater expression of anger predicted reduced BHR. These patterns were unique to the expressive facet of anger and remained after we controlled for age, gender, health status, health behaviors, social status, and reported experience of negative emotions. Implications for sociocultural modulation of bio-physiological responses are discussed.
  • 風間 みどり, 平林 秀美, Tardif Twila, 唐澤 真弓
    日本教育心理学会総会発表論文集 57 253-253 2015年  
  • Ryff CD, Miyamoto Y, Boylan JM, Coe CL, Karasawa M, Kawakami N, Kan C, Love GD, Levine C, Markus HR, Park J, Kitayama S
    Culture and brain 3(1) 1-20 2015年  査読有り
  • Katherine B. Curhan, Tamara Sims, Hazel R. Markus, Shinobu Kitayama, Mayumi Karasawa, Norito Kawakami, Gayle D. Love, Christopher L. Coe, Yuri Miyamoto, Carol D. Ryff
    PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 25(12) 2277-2280 2014年12月  査読有り
  • Katherine B. Curhan, Cynthia S. Levine, Hazel Rose Markus, Shinobu Kitayama, Jiyoung Park, Mayumi Karasawa, Norito Kawakami, Gayle D. Love, Christopher L. Coe, Yuri Miyamoto, Carol D. Ryff
    SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGICAL AND PERSONALITY SCIENCE 5(8) 855-864 2014年11月  査読有り
    Hierarchy can be conceptualized as objective social status (e.g., education level) or subjective social status (i.e., one's own judgment of one's status). Both forms predict well-being. This is the first investigation of the relative strength of these hierarchy-well-being relationships in the U.S. and Japan, cultural contexts with different normative ideas about how social status is understood and conferred. In probability samples of Japanese (N = 1,027) and U.S. (N = 1,805) adults, subjective social status more strongly predicted life satisfaction, positive affect, sense of purpose, and self-acceptance in the United States than in Japan. In contrast, objective social status more strongly predicted life satisfaction, positive relations with others, and self-acceptance in Japan than in the United States. These differences reflect divergent cultural models of self. The emphasis on independence characteristic of the United States affords credence to one's own judgment (subjective status), and the interdependence characteristic of Japan gives weight to what others can observe (objective status).
  • Igor Grossmann, Mayumi Karasawa, Chiemi Kan, Shinobu Kitayama
    Emotion 14(4) 679-692 2014年8月  査読有り
  • Chiemi Kan, Norito Kawakami, Mayumi Karasawa, Gayle Dienberg Love, Christopher L. Coe, Yuri Miyamoto, Carol D. Ryff, Shinobu Kitayama, Katherine B. Curhan, Hazel Rose Markus
    INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL MEDICINE 21(1) 53-65 2014年2月  査読有り
    Background Recently, researchers have proposed that psychological resources might be key concept in explaining the association between social class and health. However, empirical examinations of the extent to which psychological resources to social class in health are still few. Purpose This study investigated mediating effects of selected psychological resources (sense of control, self-esteem, optimism, and neuroticism) on the association of social class [education and subjective social status (SSS)] with current health status (self-rated health and the number of chronic conditions). Method This sample consisted of 1,805 Americans (818 males and 987 females) from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) survey, 2004-2006 and 1,027 Japanese (505 males and 522 females) from the Midlife in Japan (MIDJA) survey in Tokyo, Japan, 2008-2010. Information on social class, psychological resources, and health status was obtained using telephone interviews or written questionnaires. Results A mediation analysis was conducted separately for males and females in Japan and the USA. Neuroticism significantly mediated the association of education and SSS with self-rated health and chronic conditions among males and females in both countries, with one exception (not for chronic conditions among Japanese females). Sense of control significantly mediated the association of education and SSS with self-rated health among males and females in both countries. As hypothesized, self-esteem significantly mediated almost all of the associations of education and SSS with self-rated health and chronic conditions among men and women in the USA, but very few such associations in Japan. Optimism significantly mediated most associations of social class and health status in both countries, but only among females. Conclusions Overall, the findings underscore important culture- and gender specificity in the ways in which psychosocial resources mediate the links between social class and health.
  • Michael Boiger, Derya Güngör, Mayumi Karasawa, Batja Mesquita
    Cognition and Emotion 28(7) 1255-1269 2014年  査読有り
  • Jiyoung Park, Shinobu Kitayama, Hazel R. Markus, Christopher L. Coe, Yuri Miyamoto, Mayumi Karasawa, Katherine B. Curhan, Gayle D. Love, Norito Kawakami, Jennifer Morozink Boylan, Carol D. Ryff
    Emotion 13(6) 1122-1131 2013年12月  査読有り
    Individuals with lower social status have been reported to express more anger, but this evidence comes mostly from Western cultures. Here, we used representative samples of American and Japanese adults and tested the hypothesis that the association between social status and anger expression depends on whether anger serves primarily to vent frustration, as in the United States, or to display authority, as in Japan. Consistent with the assumption that lower social standing is associated with greater frustration stemming from life adversities and blocked goals, Americans with lower social status expressed more anger, with the relationship mediated by the extent of frustration. In contrast, consistent with the assumption that higher social standing affords a privilege to display anger, Japanese with higher social status expressed more anger, with the relationship mediated by decision-making authority. As expected, anger expression was predicted by subjective social status among Americans and by objective social status among Japanese. Implications for the dynamic construction of anger and anger expression are discussed. © 2013 American Psychological Association.
  • Yuri Miyamoto, Jennifer Morozink Boylan, Christopher L. Coe, Katherine B. Curhan, Cynthia S. Levine, Hazel Rose Markus, Jiyoung Park, Shinobu Kitayama, Norito Kawakami, Mayumi Karasawa, Gayle D. Love, Carol D. Ryff
    BRAIN BEHAVIOR AND IMMUNITY 34 79-85 2013年11月  査読有り
    Previous studies conducted in Western cultures have shown that negative emotions predict higher levels of pro-inflammatory biomarkers, specifically interleukin-6 (IL-6). This link between negative emotions and IL-6 may be specific to Western cultures where negative emotions are perceived to be problematic and thus may not extend to Eastern cultures where negative emotions are seen as acceptable and normal. Using samples of 1044 American and 382 Japanese middle-aged and older adults, we investigated whether the relationship between negative emotions and IL-6 varies by cultural context. Negative emotions predicted higher IL-6 among American adults, whereas no association was evident among Japanese adults. Furthermore, the interaction between culture and negative emotions remained even after controlling for demographic variables, psychological factors (positive emotions, neuroticism, extraversion), health behaviors (smoking status, alcohol consumption), and health status (chronic conditions, BMI). These findings highlight the role of cultural context in shaping how negative emotions affect inflammatory physiology and underscore the importance of cultural ideas and practices relevant to negative emotions for understanding of the interplay between psychology, physiology, and health. (C) 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
  • Jiyoung Park, Shinobu Kitayama, Mayumi Karasawa, Katherine Curhan, Hazel R. Markus, Norito Kawakami, Yuri Miyamoto, Gayle D. Love, Christopher L. Coe, Carol D. Ryff
    JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 18(2) 226-235 2013年2月  査読有り
    Although it is commonly assumed that social support positively predicts health, the empirical evidence has been inconsistent. We argue that three moderating factors must be considered: (1) support-approving norms (cultural context); (2) support-requiring situations (stressful events); and (3) support-accepting personal style (low neuroticism). Our large-scale cross-cultural survey of Japanese and US adults found significant associations between perceived support and health. The association was more strongly evident among Japanese (from a support-approving cultural context) who reported high life stress (in a support-requiring situation). Moreover, the link between support and health was especially pronounced if these Japanese were low in neuroticism.
  • 風間 みどり, 平林 秀美, 唐澤 真弓
    日本教育心理学会総会発表論文集 55 591-591 2013年  
  • Igor Grossmann, Mayumi Karasawa, Satoko Izumi, Jinkyung Na, Michael E. W. Varnum, Shinobu Kitayama, Richard E. Nisbett
    PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 23(10) 1059-1066 2012年10月  査読有り
    People from different cultures vary in the ways they approach social conflicts, with Japanese being more motivated to maintain interpersonal harmony and avoid conflicts than Americans are. Such cultural differences have developmental consequences for reasoning about social conflict. In the study reported here, we interviewed random samples of Americans from the Midwest United States and Japanese from the larger Tokyo area about their reactions to stories of intergroup and interpersonal conflicts. Responses showed that wisdom (e.g., recognition of multiple perspectives, the limits of personal knowledge, and the importance of compromise) increased with increasing age among Americans, but older age was not associated with wiser responses among Japanese. Younger and middle-aged Japanese showed greater use of wise-reasoning strategies than younger and middle-aged Americans did. This cultural difference was weaker for older participants' reactions to interpersonal conflicts and was actually reversed for intergroup conflicts. This research has important implications for the study of aging, cultural psychology, and wisdom.
  • Sheryl L. Olson, Twila Z. Tardif, Alison Miller, Barbara Felt, Adam S. Grabell, Daniel Kessler, Li Wang, Mayumi Karasawa, Hidemi Hirabayashi
    JOURNAL OF ABNORMAL CHILD PSYCHOLOGY 39(8) 1163-1175 2011年11月  査読有り
    We examined associations between child inhibitory control, harsh parental discipline and externalizing problems in 120 4 year-old boys and girls in the US, China, and Japan. Individual differences in children's inhibitory control abilities, assessed using behavioral tasks and maternal ratings, were related to child externalizing problems reported by mothers. As predicted, both child inhibitory control and maternal harsh discipline made significant contributions to child externalizing problems in all three countries. Across countries, child inhibitory control and maternal harsh discipline made significant independent contributions to early externalizing problems, suggesting an additive model of association. Our findings supported the cross-cultural generalizability of child inhibitory control and parental harsh punishment as key contributors to disruptive behavior in young children.
  • Christopher L. Coe, Gayle D. Love, Mayumi Karasawa, Norito Kawakami, Shinobu Kitayama, Hazel R. Markus, Russell P. Tracy, Carol D. Ryff
    BRAIN BEHAVIOR AND IMMUNITY 25(3) 494-502 2011年3月  査読有り
    The pleiotropic cytokine, interleukin-6 (IL-6), has emerged as a key factor in the biology of aging and the physiology of inflammation. Yet much of what we know about the normal functioning of IL-6 has been generated primarily from research on European populations and Americans of European descent. Our analyses compared IL-6 levels in 382 middle-aged and older Japanese to the values found in 1209 Caucasian- and African-Americans from the Midlife in the United States survey (MIDUS). Across the life span from 30 to 80 years of age, mean IL-6 levels were strikingly lower in Japanese individuals. Significantly lower levels of C-reactive protein (CRP) and fibrinogen (FBG) provided confirmatory evidence for a population difference in proinflammatory activity. Because IL-6 release has been associated with obesity, differences in body mass index (BMI) were taken into consideration. Japanese had the lowest, and African-Americans had the highest overall BMIs, but significant group differences in IL-6 persisted even after BMI was included as a covariate in the analyses. Additional support for distinct variation in IL-6 biology was generated when systemic levels of the soluble receptor for IL-6 (sIL-6r) were evaluated. Serum sIL-6r was higher in Japanese than Americans, but was most notably low in African-Americans. Our cytokine data concur with national differences in the prevalence of age-related illnesses linked to inflammatory physiology, including cardiovascular disease. The findings also highlight the importance of broadening the diversity of people included in population studies of health and aging, especially given the relative paucity of information for some Asian countries and on individuals of Asian heritage living in the US. (C) 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
  • Mayumi Karasawa, Katherine B. Curhan, Hazel Rose Markus, Shinobu S. Kitayama, Gayle Dienberg Love, Barry T. Radler, Carol D. Ryff
    INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF AGING & HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 73(1) 73-98 2011年  査読有り
    This study investigated age differences in multiple aspects of psychological well-being among midlife and older adults in Japan (N = 482) and the United States (N = 3,032) to test the hypothesis that older Japanese adults would rate aspects of their well-being (personal growth, purpose in life, positive relations with others) more highly that older U.S. adults. Partial support was found: older adults in Japan showed higher scores on personal growth compared to midlife adults, whereas the opposite age pattern was found in the United States. However, purpose in life showed lower scores for older adults in both cultural contexts. Interpersonal well-being, as hypothesized, was rated significantly higher, relative to the overall well-being, among Japanese compared to U.S. respondents, but only among younger adults. Women in both cultures showed higher interpersonal well-being, but also greater negative affect compared with men. Suggestions for future inquiries to advance understanding of aging and well-being in distinct cultural contexts are detailed.
  • 風間 みどり, 平林 秀美, Twila Tardif, 唐澤 真弓
    日本教育心理学会総会発表論文集 52 319-319 2010年  
  • Shinobu Kitayama, Mayumi Karasawa, Katherine B. Curhan, Carol D. Ryff, Hazel Rose Markus
    FRONTIERS IN PSYCHOLOGY 1 163 2010年  査読有り
    A cross-cultural survey was used to examine two hypotheses designed to link culture to wellbeing and health. The first hypothesis states that people are motivated toward prevalent cultural mandates of either independence (personal control) in the United States or interdependence (relational harmony) in Japan. As predicted, Americans with compromised personal control and Japanese with strained relationships reported high perceived constraint. The second hypothesis holds that people achieve wellbeing and health through actualizing the respective cultural mandates in their modes of being. As predicted, the strongest predictor of wellbeing and health was personal control in the United States, but the absence of relational strain in Japan. All analyses controlled for age, gender, educational attainment, and personality traits. The overall pattern of findings underscores culturally distinct pathways (independent versus interdependent) in achieving the positive life outcomes.
  • Shinobu Kitayama, Hyekyung Park, A. Timur Sevincer, Mayumi Karasawa, Ayse K. Uskul
    JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 97(2) 236-255 2009年8月  査読有り
    Informed by a new theoretical framework that assigns a key role to cultural tasks (culturally prescribed means to achieve cultural mandates such as independence and interdependence) in mediating the mutual influences between culture and psychological processes, the authors predicted and found that North Americans are more likely than Western Europeans (British and Germans) to (a) exhibit focused (vs. holistic) attention, (b) experience emotions associated with independence (vs. interdependence), (c) associate happiness with personal achievement (vs. communal harmony), and (d) show an inflated symbolic self. In no cases were the 2 Western European groups significantly different from one another. All Western groups showed (e) an equally strong dispositional bias in attribution. Across all of the implicit indicators of independence, Japanese were substantially less independent (or more interdependent) than the three Western groups. An explicit self-belief measure of independence and interdependence showed an anomalous pattern. These data were interpreted to suggest that the contemporary American ethos has a significant root in both Western cultural heritage and a history of voluntary settlement. Further analysis offered unique support for the cultural task analysis.
  • 東山 薫, 小川 絢子, 平林 秀美, 郷式 徹, 柿沼 美紀, 内田 由紀子, 唐澤 真弓
    日本教育心理学会総会発表論文集 50 S102-S103 2008年  
  • 風間 みどり, 平林 秀美, Twila Tardif, 唐澤 真弓
    日本教育心理学会総会発表論文集 50 602-602 2008年  
  • 唐澤 真弓
    日本社会心理学会第45回大会論文集 610-611 2004年  

主要なMISC

 56

書籍等出版物

 10

講演・口頭発表等

 1

Works(作品等)

 1

共同研究・競争的資金等の研究課題

 29